Wednesday, December 30, 2009
10 Benefits of Reviewing
Wednesday, December 23, 2009
Kayak Essentials
Within the DVD Nick and Matt focus on the core fundamentals of effective paddling – the principles of power transfer, connection, posture and feel that underpin advanced performance
They begin by building solid foundations on flat water before moving towards the application of these fundamental skills on white water, sea and surf thus demonstrating the basis of effective performance
Key skills include:
• Core fundamentals
• Forward paddling technique
• Turning and steering skills
• Balancing and bracing
Wednesday, December 16, 2009
Climbing Wall Award Training
Course provider: Mark Tozer
Staff: Rose Powell
Locations: The Beacon Climbing Centre and Plas Power Adventure
Weather: Dry & Dusty :o)
Get in touch should you wish to do your Climbing Wall Award
Further information can be obtained by visiting the Mountain Leader Training UK website
Wednesday, December 09, 2009
Wednesday, December 02, 2009
UCLAN Staff - Afon Tanat
Friday, November 27, 2009
Cumbria in Flood (3) 2009
Wednesday, November 25, 2009
Cumbria in Flood (2) 2009
Braving the floods at Braithwaite near Keswick - Terry Dixon
The River Derwent in Cockermouth - George Johnston
A couple walking their dog through a flooded Fitz Parl in Keswick - Phil Noble
Students preparing to paddle in the park at Ambleside - Catherine Taylor
Monday, November 23, 2009
Cumbria in Flood (1) 2009
Wednesday, November 18, 2009
Adventure Sports Coaching - Canoe Week
Wednesday, November 11, 2009
MLTW Course Providers Workshop
Wednesday, November 04, 2009
Single Pitch Award Training
Further information can be obtained by visiting the Mountain Leader Training UK website
Wednesday, October 28, 2009
Developing Adaptive Expertise
It is hypothesized that if people ask themselves why a skill works or why each step is needed during its application, this question will tend to lead them to form some conceptual knowledge about the object (Hatano & Oura, 2003). This was similar to what Donald Schön (1991) called ‘reflection-in-action’ as against technical problem solving in his attempt to characterise professionals. Although experts are seldom taught conceptual knowledge in the verbalised form, they may construct it in the process of solving problems or performing tasks in the domain.
Identifying particular kinds of learning experiences that develop adaptive expertise is a serious challenge for educational researchers. Hatano and Inagaki (1992) proposed four conditions that would promote sustained comprehension activity that is likely to lead to adaptive expertise. Their proposal is based on the assumption that cognitive incongruity (a state of feeling that current comprehension is inadequate; for example, wondering why a given procedure works) induces enduring comprehension activity, including seeking further information from the outside, retrieving another piece of prior knowledge, generating new inferences, examining the compatibility of inferences more closely, and so forth. The first two of the proposed conditions are concerned with the arousal of cognitive incongruity and the last two with the elicitation of committed and persistent comprehension activity in response to induced incongruity. The four conditions are: (1) encountering fairly often a novel problem to which prior knowledge is not readily applicable or a phenomenon that disconfirms a prediction based on prior knowledge; (2) engaging in frequent dialogical interaction, such as discussion, controversy, and reciprocal teaching; (3) being free from urgent external need (e.g., material rewards or positive evaluations), and thus able to pursue comprehension even when it is time consuming; and (4) being surrounded by reference group members who value understanding.
These conditions can be rephrased in terms of the nature of the practice in which people participate. For example, when a practice is oriented toward skillfully solving a fixed class of problems (e.g., making the same products for years), participants tend not to encounter novel problems, and thus they are likely to become experts distinguished in terms of speed, accuracy, and automaticity (i.e., routine experts). In contrast, when successful participation in a practice requires meeting varied and changing demands (e.g., making new, fashionable products), participants' prior knowledge must be applied flexibly, and they are likely to acquire adaptive skills. From socio-cultural perspectives, adaptive experts may not be characterised only by their domain-specific knowledge; in order to invent new procedures, for example, in addition to deeper conceptual understanding, people have to be able to participate in discourse, offer valuable suggestions, evaluate others' suggestions, and so on.
Adaptive experts exhibit a strong proactive desire to continuously learn from their experiences, improve performance and accept that their understanding will always change (Hatano & Oura, 2003). Underpinning beliefs and attitudes, along with the behaviours that are inevitably associated with such views can be both learned and taught (Perkins & Grotzer, 1997; Perkins, Jay & Tishman, 1993b). For any leader who is more able to learn flexibly in new situations, as with any learning, the process will become natural, unconscious and automated (Shuell, 1990). Fazey et al., (2005) suggest that a person who learns how to be a good learner will eventually develop greater openness to change and become more adaptive. It is anticipated that they will become a more effective outdoor leader, particularly on expeditions, as a consequence.